• Bat study reveals secrets of the social

    From ScienceDaily@1337:3/111 to All on Thu Oct 21 21:30:30 2021
    Bat study reveals secrets of the social brain

    Date:
    October 21, 2021
    Source:
    University of California - Berkeley
    Summary:
    Neuroscientists used wireless devices to record the neural activity
    of freely interacting Egyptian fruit bats, providing researchers
    with the first glimpse into how the brains of social mammals
    process complex group interactions.



    FULL STORY ========================================================================== Whether chatting with friends at a dinner party or managing a high-stakes meeting at work, communicating with others in a group requires a complex
    set of mental tasks. Our brains must track who is speaking and what is
    being said, as well as what our relationship to that person may be --
    because, after all, we probably give the opinion of our best friend more
    weight than that of a complete stranger.


    ==========================================================================
    A study published today in the journal Science provides the first glimpse
    into how the brains of social mammals process these types of complex
    group interactions.

    In the study, neuroscientists at the University of California, Berkeley,
    used wireless neural recording devices to track the brain activity of
    Egyptian fruit bats as they freely interacted in groups and occasionally vocalized to each other through high-pitched screeches and grunts.

    "Most studies of communication, particularly vocalization, are
    typically performed with single animals or with pairs of animals,
    but basically none have been conducted in actual group settings," said
    study co-first author Maimon Rose, a graduate student in the NeuroBat
    Lab at UC Berkeley. "However, many social mammals, including humans,
    typically interact in groups. Egyptian fruit bats, specifically, like to interact within large colonies." By tracking which of the bats vocalized, while simultaneously measuring the real-time neural activity in both
    the vocalizing and the listening bats, the researchers were able to
    decode how neurons in the bats' frontal cortices distinguished among vocalizations made by themselves and by others, as well how the bats distinguished among different individuals in the group.

    When they compared the neural recordings among the different bats, they
    also found that brain activity became highly correlated when a bat made
    a vocalization. Surprisingly, they found that communication produced
    by bats that were "friendlier" -- those that spent more time in close
    proximity to others - - induced a higher degree of correlations across
    the brains of the group members.



    ========================================================================== "Other neuroscience studies have tried to examine small pieces of
    these interactions individually. For example, one study might examine
    how neurons respond when somebody else speaks, and then a separate study
    might look at how neurons respond when that individual speaks," said study senior author Michael Yartsev,an assistant professor of neurobiology
    and bioengineering at UC Berkeley. "This study is the first to really
    put all of these pieces together to get a full picture of communication
    within a social group." Thousands of squabbling roommates Like humans, Egyptian fruit bats are highly social creatures. After long nights spent
    flying 10 miles or more in search of ripe fruit, these nocturnal animals
    pass the daylight hours packed into tight caves and crevices alongside
    hundreds or thousands of other bats. Not surprisingly, studies suggest
    that these bats typically vocalize to squabble over food, sleeping space
    and mating attempts.

    "These bats are very long-lived -- they live about 25 years -- and
    basically their entire lives are spent in this group social living,"
    Yartsev said. "So, the ability to live together in a group and
    communicate with each other is an inherent feature of their lives."
    Even in laboratory settings, bats seem to prefer the comfort of a crowd, typically spending most of their time physically pressed against each
    other in a tight cluster. Notably, aside from making clicking noises for echolocation, Egyptian fruit bats do not engage in any long-distance
    form of communication and appear to vocalize to other bats only when
    clustered together.



    ==========================================================================
    "If you visit these bat caves, you can just look up and see tens of
    thousands of animals," Yartsev said. "So, it really wouldn't make sense
    for a bat to shout across to the cave to another bat." Bats' habit of
    only vocalizing within tight social clumps makes them ideal subjects for studying group communication because, if a bat does call out while in a cluster, that call is most likely an indicator that social communication
    is taking place. However, this behavior also posed one of many technical challenges for the research team, said study co-first author Boaz Styr,
    a postdoctoral researcher in the NeuroBat Lab.

    "One big problem was trying to identify which bat made a vocalization,
    because they spend their time in tight clusters and sometimes obscure each other," Styr said. "Even though we had high resolution cameras recording
    at different angles, and lots of microphones around, it could be hard to pinpoint which bat was making a call at exactly which point." During the experiments, four to eight bats were allowed to freely interact in a
    darkened enclosure in the lab, and allowed to spontaneously vocalize. To accurately identify which bat made each vocalization, the team developed wireless vibration sensors that the bats could wear around their necks,
    almost like necklaces, and which could detect the vibrations created
    when a bat made a call.

    "These vibration sensors, paired with our ability to wirelessly record
    neural data from multiple bats at the same time, allowed us to create
    this experiment in which the bats could freely behave and spontaneously communicate," Styr said. "Getting all of these technical things to
    work together was extremely challenging, but it allowed us to ask these
    very important questions." Neurons for self and others In one set of experiments, the researchers allowed groups of four or five bats to
    freely interact within a darkened enclosure in the lab, while carefully monitoring each bat's vocalizations and brain activity.

    They found that, within each bat's frontal cortex -- an area known to be involved in mediating social behaviors in animals and humans -- separate
    sets of neurons were activated, depending on which bat in the group
    vocalized; in other words, a vocalization from one bat would stimulate
    activity in one set of neurons, while a vocalization from a different
    bat would stimulate a different set of neurons. These correlations were
    so strong that, after identifying which sets of neurons corresponded to
    which bat, the researchers could identify which bat had vocalized purely
    by looking at the neural activity of the other bats.

    "What these individual neurons cared about was, 'Am I making the
    call? Or is somebody else making the call?' no matter what type of
    vocalization it was," Styr said. "Other neurons were only sensitive to
    when one specific bat within the group was talking." Earlier work from
    the NeuroBat Lab has demonstrated that the brains of bat pairs tend to
    sync up when they socialize. In this study, the authors discovered that
    during vocal communication, the whole group syncs up together.

    This effect was not observed when the bats simply heard playback of
    the same sounds, suggesting that this phenomenon was specific to active communication taking place among the group members.

    Intriguingly, the degree of correlation among the group members'
    brains appeared to depend on which bat was talking, with some bats
    having stronger synchronization with specific individuals. Remarkably,
    these inter-brain patterns lasted for weeks, presumably representing
    stable social relationships among the individuals.

    To better understand how social dynamics impact brain activity, the
    researchers conducted a separate set of experiments in which eight
    bats were allowed to freely interact in a larger enclosure. In addition
    to monitoring the vocalizations and neural activity of each bat, they
    also tracked each bat's spatial position relative to the other bats in
    the group.

    "Bats can recognize and have stable social relationships with other
    individual bats, even over long periods of time and in different circumstances," Rose said. "And because we had this group of bats, we
    decided to track their positions in a larger area to see if that would
    tell us anything about their social relationships -- who likes whom,
    and who are more sociable bats and the less sociable bats." They found
    that, while most "in-cluster" bats spent nearly all their time clumped
    together with other bats, a couple of "out-of-cluster" bats spent more
    time off to the side, separate from the group. Surprisingly, the team
    also found that the in-cluster or out-of-cluster status of a bat impacted
    the neural activity of the other bats during vocalizations.

    "We found that when thein-cluster bats vocalized, they elicited a much
    more accurate neural representation of their identity in the other bats
    and also elicited a much higher level of brain synchrony within the
    group," Rose said.

    "So, while its not entirely clear what exactly is going on, it seems
    that the behavior of the out-of-cluster bats really shifts their neural representation in the brains of the other bats." Understanding the
    neural underpinnings of why some individuals can navigate almost any
    social situation with ease, while others are consistently ostracized or misunderstood, could have major implications for improving human mental
    health, Yartsev said. He hopes the study inspires neuroscientists to
    take a more comprehensive look at group communication within other
    social mammals.

    "Often, in neuroscience, we like to take a simplified approach and focus
    on one component of a complex process at a time," Yartsev said. "But in reality, the social world is complex. When we spend time with our friends, there's a lot of relationship history and baggage that comes with each interaction: what happened yesterday, who that person is friends with,
    how each person feels in the moment. And so, breaking things down and
    looking at them individually can give an illusion of control but, in
    fact, make it very difficult to get the complete picture." "Our brains,
    and those of animals, have evolved for and constantly struggle with the complexity of real life," Yartsev added. "I personally believe that to
    truly understand the brain, we need to embrace this complexity, rather
    than fear it, and, indeed, every time we did so, we found out something
    new and exciting. I hope that this, as well as our other studies,
    demonstrate that we need to study the brain in all its complexity."
    Co-authors of the paper include Tobias A. Schmid and Julie E. Elie of
    UC Berkeley. This research was supported by the National Institutes of
    Health (Award DP2-DC016163), the National Institute of Mental Health
    (Award 1- R01MH25387-01), the New York Stem Cell Foundation (Award NYSCF-R-NI40), the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation (Award FG-2017-9646), the
    Brain Research Foundation (Award BRFSG-2017-09), the Packard Fellowship
    (Award 2017-66825), the Klingenstein Simons Fellowship, the Human
    Frontiers Science Program, the Pew Charitable Trust (Award 00029645),
    the McKnight Foundation, the Dana Foundation and the Human Frontiers
    Science Program postdoctoral fellowship.

    ========================================================================== Story Source: Materials provided by
    University_of_California_-_Berkeley. Original written by Kara Manke. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


    ========================================================================== Journal Reference:
    1. Maimon C. Rose, Boaz Styr, Tobias A. Schmid, Julie E. Elie and
    Michael M.

    Yartsev. Cortical representation of group social communication
    in bats.

    Science, 2021 DOI: 10.1126/science.aba9584 ==========================================================================

    Link to news story: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/10/211021175145.htm

    --- up 7 weeks, 8 hours, 25 minutes
    * Origin: -=> Castle Rock BBS <=- Now Husky HPT Powered! (1337:3/111)